60 pages • 2 hours read
Steven PinkerA modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.
Throughout Pinker’s discussion of how the mind works, we see that the human mind works on a rule system but is also flexible. The rules about how the smallest homunculi work appear rigid and simple, and yet the human mind can adapt to the current environment, generate new connections, learn new things, and change how the smallest homunculi respond to stimuli. The mind isn’t too flexible or we would entertain too many options with each decision or not have enough consistency to develop foundations from which to learn new things and respond to new environments. Instead, the mind has appropriate flexibility—the right balance of the ability to change and consistency in its operations.
In Chapter 2, we see this idea of appropriate flexibility in the way we can learn a new word for an object we know, and we quickly apply everything we know about the object to the new word. In Chapter 4, we see that the visual system only allows us to process information up to three dimensions, but the mind allows us to categorize an object on an almost infinite number of dimensions. The same mind, developed with the same tools, places an appropriate constraint on vision but not on conceptual information. We would not want visual information to be processed along thousands of dimensions that don’t exist or that aren’t relevant for our day-to-day existence. We also would not want conceptual information to be limited to three categories. Pinker does not directly address how we arrive at his appropriate flexibility, but as the mind developed, constraints were selected where appropriate and not selected when not appropriate.
This appropriate flexibility comes through even in larger concepts like emotion and relationships. Emotions serve multiple functions, and they can be signals of many possible situations based on the other contextual information. Each emotion, such as disgust and fear, is flexible in different ways. Disgust is ingrained in us early and difficult to change even when we learn there is nothing disgusting about an item. We learn to avoid “disgusting” foods that others will readily eat because in our history, that item may have been potentially deadly. Once learned, such feelings are difficult to change. We may work up the courage to try something considered disgusting, but it’s usually a one-time attempt or occurs out of necessity. These items were linked to poor survival in our past, and that link should be difficult to remove.
Fear, on the other hand, is more flexible. Some fears seen across cultures, like spiders and snakes, are linked to our ancestors’ survival and still seen today even though they are less relevant in our environment. However, fear can also be trained. The ability to adapt fear makes sense as we need to learn to fear things that are dangerous in a new environment. If we maintained all our fears from living in the jungle to living on the savannah and never learned what was dangerous in the new environment, we would not have lasted long. Again, the mind has evolved appropriate flexibility for processing information to maximize survival and adaptability.
Pinker starts in Chapter 1 showing us that the mind builds smaller pieces into larger pieces and smaller concepts into larger concepts. The process for doing so is iterative in that we seem to follow the same basic procedures at the smallest level that are followed at higher levels. We repeat the process to build up to complex thoughts, ideas, and actions. This iteration is important because it gives the brain one of its most interesting qualities: growth. The mind does not grow physically beyond what we know of brain growth, but the mind grows exponentially throughout the lifespan in the connections it makes, the knowledge is maintains, and the solutions it produces in response to problems. We have seemingly infinite information-processing capacity that hinges on the iterative process by which we make sense of information.
As Pinker discusses in Chapter 5, we learn abstract concepts by building off smaller, concrete concepts. We learn math first with numbers and basic number theory, which we have evidence infants have, and then we expand to integrals and vectors in calculus and beyond. These larger, abstract concepts are fragile and require practice to maintain and use regularly with ease. Without appropriate practice and use (like a muscle), these concepts will fade, but the basic number theory will remain. Even in relationships we can see this iterative process.
We approach our relationships with friends as though they are unrelated family members, looking for similar behaviors and interactions that show us a person can be trustworthy and helpful. We start small with concrete and easy gestures and requests, and as the friendship grows, we develop a complex understanding of our mutual reliance. This iterative process is at the core of how the mind works and extends up to its most complex abilities.
The elegance of the mind has been espoused by many, from scientists to religious authorities, all of whom claim that the human mind is a sign of whatever they believe organizes or controls the universe. Pinker does not use the word elegance or approach the topic the same way as others, but he still invokes the idea that the mind is too perfect to have been created by anything other than natural selection. With each connection he makes between the human experience and his argument for how the mind works, he reminds us that it all fits far too perfectly to have any other explanation of its origins. Pinker differs from others who have invoked the same argument because of the breadth of his examples. The elegance Pinker describes shows the connection in how the mind works all the way from the connections among neurons to the way you think about your father, brother, friend, and neighbor.
Elegance is often invoked by others to imply we shouldn’t try to understand the mind because it is somehow beyond us. It came from something we can’t understand so we should leave it at that. Pinker argues the exact opposite, that the elegance of the mind means there is structure and process and a system behind the mind that can be studied and explained. Pinker argues the elegance of the mind is exactly why we should study it. For Pinker, the elegance is what makes the mind make sense, even though he still marvels at times at the amazing abilities and organization of the mind. Because the mind itself is so elegant and structured, the system that created it is likely also highly elegant and structured. Natural selection fits that bill, and his examples show us that natural selection is simple but powerful. The elegance of the mind comes from the fact that it was shaped by a simple and powerful process instead of by an elaborate, difficult, and complex process.
By Steven Pinker